Drive reduction theory (learning theory)

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

Drive reduction theory, developed by Clark Hull in 1943, is a major theory of motivation in the behaviorist learning theory tradition.[1] "Drive" is defined as motivation that arises due to a psychological or physiological need.[2] It works as an internal stimulus that motivates an individual to sate the drive.[3] It has also been described as an internal and instinctual process that moves individuals to take actions that would allow them to attain their desired goal or end-state.[4]

Details[]

According to such theorists as Clark Hull and Kenneth Spence, drive reduction is a major force behind learning and behavior. Primary drives are innate drives (e.g., thirst, hunger, and sex), whereas secondary drives are learned by conditioning (e.g., money). Doris Kraeling and Byron Campbell conducted experiments to determine if “reduction would be more effective as a reinforcer if the initial drive were low than if the initial drive were high.” Their findings were that “changes in stimuli are more discriminable at low levels of stimulus intensity than at higher levels of stimulus intensity.” [5] Multiple drives result when an organism is faced with more than one need simultaneously. Research has shown that this condition affects learning.[6]

There are also the complications to drive reduction theory caused by so-called "pleasure-seeking" behaviors, which seem to be contradictory to the theory's precepts. Why would an individual actively seek out more stimulation if it is already in a state of relaxation and fulfillment? A good example is when an individual leaves home to go to a potentially dangerous carnival. There is no base physiological drive to go to the carnival, but the individual exhausts resources to go there. Judson Brown attempts to explain this phenomenon, saying, "the sensory consequences of most responses are practically never intense enough to provide increments to the drive level."[7]

Motivational readiness[]

An expansion on the drive-incentive link, developed by Warden, states that an individual's physiological needs will be coupled with a proportionate drive.[3] Affordances are the available resources present in an individual's environment; these would be at their disposal to use to obtain their desired end-state. The individual's environment, in which the affordances are located, is similar to Lewin's living space.[8] The level of effectiveness and affordance depends on what the want is. The affordance properties need to be able to suit the characteristics needed for the want to be fulfilled.[9] For example, an individual whose want is shelter from a hail storm would not be satisfied if the affordance given was a miniature sized bag of gummy worms. Additionally, based on Warden's drive-incentive link, as either the drive or incentive increases, the behavior also increases.[10] According to Hull, due to the interconnected properties of drive and motivation, in the presence of a drive, the discomfort of the individual increases; this discomfort gives them the motivation to dispel the need at the core of the drive.[9]

The emergence of the theory of motivational readiness comes from previous attempts to explain how internal and external sources interact to influence motivation and behavior. This theory is dependent on the notion that individuals will have a want and that they will take actions to obtain the want (assuming that is actually obtainable).[11] Wants can be any physiological or psychological need, such as the need for food; as an example, an individual can drive to Waffle House with the expectation that their hunger will be satiated by the fluffy omelets there.

Motivational readiness has been prominent in studies involving exercise,[12] weight control, diet, and smoking.[13][14]

See also[]

References[]

  1. ^ Dewey, R. (2007). Psychology: An introduction. Retrieved from http://www.intropsych.com/index.html
  2. ^ Hull, C.L. (1952). A behavior system. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
  3. ^ a b Deckers, Lambert (2018). Motivation Biological, Psychological, and Environmental. New York, NY: Routledge. pp. 57–60. ISBN 978-1-138-03632-1.
  4. ^ Lambert, Deckers, (2018). Motivation biological, psychological, and environmental (5th edition ed.). New York: Routledge. pp. 42–43. ISBN 9781351713887. OCLC 1022784633.
  5. ^ Campbell, BA; Kraeling, D (February 1953). "Response strength as a function of drive level and amount of drive reduction". Journal of Experimental Psychology. 45 (2): 97–101. doi:10.1037/h0060223. PMID 13052836.
  6. ^ Wike, EL; Barrientos, G (October 1958). "Secondary reinforcement and multiple drive reduction". Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology. 51 (5): 640–643. doi:10.1037/h0047699. PMID 13587701.
  7. ^ Brown, Judson S. (1955). "Pleasure-seeking behavior and the drive-reduction hypothesis". Psychological Review. 62 (3): 169–179. doi:10.1037/h0047034. PMID 14371895.
  8. ^ Lewin, Kurt (1936). Principles of topological psychology. McGraw-Hill. doi:10.1037/10019-000. OCLC 916125511.[page needed]
  9. ^ a b Deckers, Lambert (2018). Motivation Biological, Psychological, and Environmental. New York, NY: Routledge. pp. 50–53. ISBN 978-1-138-03632-1.
  10. ^ Warden, Carl John (1931). "The Columbia Obstruction Method". Animal Motivation: Experimental Studies on the Albino Rat. Columbia University Press. pp. 3–16. ISBN 978-0-598-38190-3.
  11. ^ Kruglanski, Arie W.; Chernikova, Marina; Rosenzweig, Emily; Kopetz, Catalina (July 2014). "On motivational readiness". Psychological Review. 121 (3): 367–388. doi:10.1037/a0037013. PMID 25090424.
  12. ^ Pinto, Bernardine M.; Lynn, Henry; Marcus, Bess H.; DePue, Judith; Goldstein, Michael G. (1 February 2001). "Physician-based activity counseling: Intervention effects on mediators of motivational readiness for physical activity". Annals of Behavioral Medicine. 23 (1): 2–10. doi:10.1207/S15324796ABM2301_2. PMID 11302351. S2CID 3681805.
  13. ^ Marcus, Bess H.; Rakowski, William; Rossi, Joseph S. (1992). "Assessing motivational readiness and decision making for exercise". Health Psychology. 11 (4): 257–261. doi:10.1037/0278-6133.11.4.257. PMID 1396494.
  14. ^ Bock, B. C.; Marcus, B. H.; Rossi, J. S.; Redding, C. A. (1998). "Motivational readiness for change: Diet, exercise, and smoking". American Journal of Health Behavior. 22 (4): 248–258.

Further reading[]

  • Wolpe, J (January 1950). "Need-reduction, drive-reduction, and reinforcement; a neurophysiological view". Psychological Review. 57 (1): 19–26. doi:10.1037/h0055810. PMID 15409077.
  • Seward, JP (May 1956). "Drive, incentive, and reinforcement". Psychological Review. 63 (3): 195–203. doi:10.1037/h0048229. PMID 13323175.
  • Mowrer, OH; Solomon, LN (March 1954). "Contiguity vs. drive-reduction in conditioned fear: the proximity and abruptness of drive-reduction". The American Journal of Psychology. 67 (1): 15–25. doi:10.2307/1418068. JSTOR 1418068. PMID 13138766.
  • Leuba, Clarence (1 March 1955). "Toward Some Integration of Learning Theories: The Concept of Optimal Stimulation". Psychological Reports. 1 (1): 27–33. doi:10.2466/pr0.1955.1.g.27. S2CID 144546093.
  • Harlow, HF (January 1953). "Mice, monkeys, men, and motives". Psychological Review. 60 (1): 23–32. doi:10.1037/h0056040. PMID 13037933.
Retrieved from ""