Listen to this article

Learned helplessness

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

Learned helplessness is behavior exhibited by a subject after enduring repeated aversive stimuli beyond their control. It was initially thought to be caused from the subject's acceptance of their powerlessness: discontinuing attempts to escape or avoid the aversive stimulus, even when such alternatives are unambiguously presented. Upon exhibiting such behavior, the subject was said to have acquired learned helplessness.[1][2] Over the past few decades, neuroscience has provided insight into learned helplessness and shown that the original theory actually had it backwards: the brain's default state is to assume that control is not present, and the presence of "helpfulness" is what is actually learned first. However, it is unlearned when a subject is faced with prolonged aversive stimulation.[3]

In humans, learned helplessness is related to the concept of self-efficacy; the individual's belief in their innate ability to achieve goals. Learned helplessness theory is the view that clinical depression and related mental illnesses may result from such real or perceived absence of control over the outcome of a situation.[4]

Foundation of research and theory[]

Early experiments[]

Inescapable shock training in the shuttle box

American psychologist Martin Seligman initiated research on learned helplessness in 1967 at the University of Pennsylvania as an extension of his interest in depression.[5] This research was later expanded through experiments by Seligman and others. One of the first was an experiment by Seligman & Overmier: In Part 1 of this study, three groups of dogs were placed in harnesses. Group 1 dogs were simply put in a harness for a period of time and were later released. Groups 2 and 3 consisted of "yoked pairs". Dogs in Group 2 were given electric shocks at random times, which the dog could end by pressing a lever. Each dog in Group 3 was paired with a Group 2 dog; whenever a Group 2 dog got a shock, its paired dog in Group 3 got a shock of the same intensity and duration, but its lever did not stop the shock. To a dog in Group 3, it seemed that the shock ended at random because it was their paired dog in Group 2 that was causing it to stop. Thus, for Group 3 dogs, the shock was "inescapable".

In Part 2 of the experiment, the same three groups of dogs were tested in a shuttle-box apparatus (a chamber containing two rectangular compartments divided by a barrier a few inches high). All of the dogs could escape shocks on one side of the box by jumping over a low partition to the other side. The dogs in Groups 1 and 2 quickly learned this task and escaped the shock. Most of the Group 3 dogs – which had previously learned that nothing they did had any effect on shocks – simply lay down passively and whined when they were shocked.[5]

In a second experiment later that year with new groups of dogs, Maier and Seligman ruled out the possibility that, instead of learned helplessness, the Group 3 dogs failed to avert in the second part of the test because they had learned some behavior that interfered with "escape". To prevent such interfering behavior, Group 3 dogs were immobilized with a paralyzing drug (curare) and underwent a procedure similar to that in Part 1 of the Seligman and Overmier experiment. When tested as before in Part 2, these Group 3 dogs exhibited helplessness as before. This result serves as an indicator for the ruling out of the interference hypothesis.

From these experiments, it was thought that there was to be only one cure for helplessness. In Seligman's hypothesis, the dogs do not try to escape because they expect that nothing they do will stop the shock. To change this expectation, experimenters physically picked up the dogs and moved their legs, replicating the actions the dogs would need to take in order to escape from the electrified grid. This had to be done at least twice before the dogs would start willfully jumping over the barrier on their own. In contrast, threats, rewards, and observed demonstrations had no effect on the "helpless" Group 3 dogs.[5][6][full citation needed]

Later experiments[]

Later experiments have served to confirm the depressive effect of feeling a lack of control over an aversive stimulus. For example, in one experiment, humans performed mental tasks in the presence of distracting noise. Those who could use a switch to turn off the noise rarely bothered to do so, yet they performed better than those who could not turn off the noise. Simply being aware of this option was enough to substantially counteract the noise effect.[7] In 2011, an animal study[8] found that animals with control over stressful stimuli exhibited changes in the excitability of certain neurons in the prefrontal cortex. Animals that lacked control failed to exhibit this neural effect and showed signs consistent with learned helplessness and social anxiety.

Expanded theories[]

Research has found that a human's reaction to feeling a lack of control differs both between individuals and between situations, i.e. learned helplessness sometimes remains specific to one situation but at other times generalizes across situations.[7][9][10] Such variations are not explained by the original theory of learned helplessness, and an influential view is that such variations depend on an individual's attributional or explanatory style.[11] According to this view, how someone interprets or explains adverse events affects their likelihood of acquiring learned helplessness and subsequent depression.[12] For example, people with pessimistic explanatory style tend to see negative events as permanent ("it will never change"), personal ("it's my fault"), and pervasive ("I can't do anything correctly"), and are likely to suffer from learned helplessness and depression.[13]

In 1978, Lyn Yvonne Abramson, Seligman and John D. Teasdale reformulated Seligman's work, using attribution theory. They proposed that people differed in how they classified negative experiences on three scales, from internal to external, stable to unstable, and from global to specific. They believed that people who were more likely to attribute negative events to internal, stable, and global causes were more likely to become depressed than those attributed things to causes at the other ends of the scales.[14]

Bernard Weiner proposed a detailed account of the attributional approach to learned helplessness in 1986. His attribution theory includes the dimensions of globality/specificity, stability/instability, and internality/externality:[15]

  • A global attribution occurs when the individual believes that the cause of negative events is consistent across different contexts.
    • A specific attribution occurs when the individual believes that the cause of a negative event is unique to a particular situation.
  • A stable attribution occurs when the individual believes the cause to be consistent across time.
    • An unstable attribution occurs when the individual thinks that the cause is specific to one point in time.
  • An external attribution assigns causality to situational or external factors,
    • while an internal attribution assigns causality to factors within the person.[12]

Research has shown that those with an internal, stable, and global attributional style for negative events can be more at risk for a depressive reaction to failure experiences.

Neurobiological perspective[]

Research has shown that increased 5-HT (serotonin) activity in the dorsal raphe nucleus plays a critical role in learned helplessness. Other key brain regions that are involved with the expression of helpless behavior include the basolateral amygdala, central nucleus of the amygdala and bed nucleus of the stria terminalis.[16] Activity in medial prefrontal cortex, dorsal hippocampus, septum and hypothalamus has also been observed during states of helplessness.

In the article, "Exercise, Learned Helplessness, and the Stress-Resistant Brain", Benjamin N. Greenwood and Monika Fleshner discuss how exercise might prevent stress-related disorders such as anxiety and depression. They show evidence that running wheel exercise prevents learned helplessness behaviors in rats.[17] They suggest that the amount of exercise may not be as important as simply exercising at all. The article also discusses the neurocircuitry of learned helplessness, the role of serotonin (or 5-HT), and the exercise-associated neural adaptations that may contribute to the stress-resistant brain. However, the authors finally conclude that "The underlying neurobiological mechanisms of this effect, however, remain unknown. Identifying the mechanisms by which exercise prevents learned helplessness could shed light on the complex neurobiology of depression and anxiety and potentially lead to novel strategies for the prevention of stress-related mood disorders".[18]

Health implications[]

People who perceive events as uncontrollable show a variety of symptoms that threaten their mental and physical well-being. They experience stress, they often show disruption of emotions demonstrating passivity or aggressivity, and they can also have difficulty performing cognitive tasks such as problem-solving.[19][20][21] They are less likely to change unhealthy patterns of behavior, causing them, for example, to neglect diet, exercise, and medical treatment.[22][23]

Depression[]

Abnormal and cognitive psychologists have found a strong correlation between depression-like symptoms and learned helplessness in laboratory animals.[24]

Young adults and middle-aged parents with a pessimistic explanatory style often suffer from depression.[25] They tend to be poor at problem-solving and cognitive restructuring, and also tend to demonstrate poor job satisfaction and interpersonal relationships in the workplace.[22][26] Those with a pessimistic style also tend to have weakened immune systems, having not only increased vulnerability to minor ailments (e.g., cold, fever) and major illness (e.g., heart attack, cancers), but also poorer recovery from health problems.[27]

Social impact[]

Learned helplessness can be a factor in a wide range of social situations.

  • In emotionally abusive relationships, the victim often develops learned helplessness. This occurs when the victim confronts or tries to leave the abuser only to have the abuser dismiss or trivialize the victim's feelings, pretend to care but not change, or impede the victim from leaving. Unfortunately, as the situation continues and the abuse gets worse, the victim will begin to give up and show signs of this learned helplessness.[28]
  • The motivational effect of learned helplessness is often seen in the classroom. Students who repeatedly fail may conclude that they are incapable of improving their performance, and this attribution keeps them from trying to succeed, which results in increased helplessness, continued failure, loss of self-esteem and other social consequences. This becomes a pattern that will spiral downward if it continues to go untreated.[29][30]
  • Child abuse by neglect can be a manifestation of learned helplessness. For example, when parents believe they are incapable of stopping an infant's crying, they may simply give up trying to do anything for the child. This learned helplessness will negatively impact both the parent and child. [31]
  • Those who are extremely shy or anxious in social situations may become passive due to feelings of helplessness[citation needed]. Gotlib and Beatty (1985) found that people who cite helplessness in social settings may be viewed poorly by others, which tends to reinforce passivity.
  • Aging individuals may respond with helplessness to the deaths of friends and family members, the loss of jobs and income, and the development of age-related health problems. This may cause them to neglect their medical care, financial affairs, and other important needs.[32]
  • According to Cox et al., Abramson, Devine, and Hollon (2012), learned helplessness is a key factor in depression that is caused by inescapable prejudice (i.e., "deprejudice").[33] Thus: "Helplessness born in the face of inescapable prejudice matches the helplessness born in the face of inescapable shocks."[34]
  • According to Ruby K. Payne's book A Framework for Understanding Poverty, treatment of the poor can lead to a cycle of poverty, a culture of poverty, and generational poverty. This type of learned helplessness is passed from parents to children. People who embrace this mentality feel there is no way to escape poverty and so one must live in the moment and not plan for the future, trapping families in poverty.[35]

Social problems resulting from learned helplessness may seem unavoidable to those entrenched. However, there are various ways to reduce or prevent it. When induced in experimental settings, learned helplessness has been shown to resolve itself with the passage of time.[36] People can be immunized against the perception that events are uncontrollable by increasing their awareness of previous experiences, when they were able to affect the desired outcome.[37] Cognitive therapy can be used to show people that their actions do make a difference[38] and bolster their self-esteem. Seeking out these types of treatment options can be extremely helpful for people stuck in a rut when it comes to learned helplessness. While it may initially feel hard to escape, with the proper time and help it can get better. [39]

Extensions[]

Cognitive scientist and usability engineer Donald Norman used learned helplessness to explain why people blame themselves when they have a difficult time using simple objects in their environment.[40]

The UK educationalist Phil Bagge describes it as a learning avoidance strategy caused by prior failure and the positive reinforcement of avoidance such as asking teachers or peers to explain and consequently do the work. It shows itself as sweet helplessness or aggressive helplessness often seen in challenging problem solving contexts, such as learning to use a new computer programming language.[41]

The US sociologist Harrison White has suggested in his book Identity and Control that the notion of learned helplessness can be extended beyond psychology into the realm of social action. When a culture or political identity fails to achieve desired goals, perceptions of collective ability suffer.

The Unabomber, Ted Kaczynski, used the concept of learned helplessness to mass-diagnose the psychological problems that are prevalent in modern society, such as depression, anxiety, insomnia, and eating disorders. He also attributed to rise of "leftist" social movements to an epidemic of learned helplessness.[42]

Emergence under torture[]

Studies on learned helplessness served as the basis for developing enhanced interrogation techniques. In CIA interrogation manuals, learned helplessness is characterized as "apathy" which may result from prolonged use of coercive techniques which result in a "debility-dependency-dread" state in the subject, "If the debility-dependency-dread state is unduly prolonged, however, the arrestee may sink into a defensive apathy from which it is hard to arouse him."[43][44]

See also[]

References[]

  1. ^ Carlson, Neil R. (2010). Psychology the science of behavior. Pearson Canada. p. 409. ISBN 978-0-205-69918-6.
  2. ^ Nolen, J.L. "Learned helplessness". Encyclopædia Britannica. Retrieved 14 January 2014.
  3. ^ Maier, Steven F.; Seligman, Martin E. P. (July 2016). "Learned helplessness at fifty: Insights from neuroscience". Psychological Review. 123 (4): 349–367. doi:10.1037/rev0000033. ISSN 1939-1471. PMC 4920136. PMID 27337390.
  4. ^ Seligman, M. E. P. (1975). Helplessness: On Depression, Development, and Death. San Francisco: W. H. Freeman. ISBN 978-0-7167-2328-8.
  5. ^ Jump up to: a b c Seligman, M. E. P. (1972). "Learned helplessness". Annual Review of Medicine. 23 (1): 407–412. doi:10.1146/annurev.me.23.020172.002203. PMID 4566487.
  6. ^ Seligman, M. E. P., 1975 Scientific American
  7. ^ Jump up to: a b Hiroto, D.S.; Seligman, M.E.P. (1975). "Generality of learned helplessness in man". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 31 (2): 311–27. doi:10.1037/h0076270.
  8. ^ Varela, Juan; Wang, Jungang; Varnell, Andrew; Cooper, Donald (2011). "Control over stress induces plasticity of individual prefrontal cortical neurons: A conductance-based neural simulation". Nature Precedings. doi:10.1038/npre.2011.6267.1.
  9. ^ Peterson, C.; Park, C. (1998). "Learned helplessness and explanatory style". In Barone, D. F.; Hersen, M.; VanHasselt, V. B. (eds.). Advanced Personality. New York: Plenum Press. pp. 287–308. ISBN 978-0-306-45745-6.
  10. ^ Cole, C. S.; Coyne, J. C. (1977). "Situational specificity of laboratory-induced learned helplessness in humans". Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 86 (6): 615–623. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.86.6.615.
  11. ^ Peterson, C.; Seligman, M.E.P. (1984). "Causal explanations as a risk factor for depression: Theory and evidence". Psychological Review. 91 (3): 347–74. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.91.3.347. PMID 6473583. S2CID 32863814.
  12. ^ Jump up to: a b Abramson, L. Y.; Seligman, M. E. P.; Teasdale, J. D. (1978). "Learned helplessness in humans: Critique and reformulation". Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 87 (1): 49–74. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.87.1.49. PMID 649856. S2CID 2845204.
  13. ^ Peterson, C.; Maier, S. F.; Seligman, M. E. P. (1995). Learned Helplessness: A Theory for the Age of Personal Control. New York: Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0-19-504467-6.
  14. ^ Abramson, L. Y.; Seligman, M. E.; Teasdale, J. D. (February 1978). "Learned helplessness in humans: critique and reformulation". Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 87 (1): 49–74. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.87.1.49. ISSN 0021-843X. PMID 649856.
  15. ^ Weiner, B. (1986). An attributional theory of motivation and emotion. New York: Springer-Verlag.
  16. ^ Hammack, Sayamwong; Cooper, Matthew; Lezak, Kimberly (2012). "Overlapping neurobiology of learned helplessness and conditioned defeat: Implications for PTSD and mood disorders". Original Paper. University of Vermont. 62 (2): 565–575. doi:10.1016/j.neuropharm.2011.02.024. PMC 3433056. PMID 21396383.
  17. ^ idem, p82
  18. ^ Greenwood, Benjamin; Fleshner, Monika (2008). "Exercise, Learned Helplessness, and the Stress-Resistant Brain" (PDF). Original Paper. University of Colorado-Boulder and Department of Integrative Physiology. 10 (2): 81–98. doi:10.1007/s12017-008-8029-y. PMID 18300002. S2CID 2235604. Archived from the original (PDF) on 23 June 2011. Retrieved 2 November 2012.
  19. ^ Roth, S. (1980). "A revised model of learned helplessness in humans". Journal of Personality. 48 (1): 103–33. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.1980.tb00969.x. PMID 7365672.
  20. ^ Wortman, Camille B.; Brehm, Jack W. (1975). "Responses to Uncontrollable Outcomes: An Integration of Reactance Theory and the Learned Helplessness Model". In Berkowitz, Leonard (ed.). Advances in Experimental Social Psychology. 8. pp. 277–336. doi:10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60253-1. ISBN 9780120152087.
  21. ^ Sullivan, D.R.; Liu, X; Corwin, D.S. (2012). "Learned Helplessness Among Families and Surrogate Decision-makers of Patients Admitted to Medical, Surgical and Trauma Intensive Care Units". Chest. 142 (6): 1440–1446. doi:10.1378/chest.12-0112. PMC 3515025. PMID 22661454.
  22. ^ Jump up to: a b Henry, P.C. (2005). "Life stress, explanatory style, hopelessness, and occupational stress". International Journal of Stress Management. 12 (3): 241–56. doi:10.1037/1072-5245.12.3.241.
  23. ^ Jones, Ishmael (2008, revised 2010). The Human Factor: Inside the CIA's Dysfunctional Intelligence Culture. New York: Encounter Books. ISBN 978-1-59403-223-3.
  24. ^ Maier, Steven F.; Watkins, Linda R. (2005). "Stressor controllability and learned helplessness: The roles of the dorsal raphe nucleus, serotonin, and corticotropin-releasing factor" (PDF). Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 29 (4–5): 829–841. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.03.021. PMID 15893820. S2CID 7340885. Archived from the original (PDF) on 18 September 2018.
  25. ^ Chang, E.C.; Sanna, L.J. (2007). "Affectivity and psychological adjustment across two adult generations: Does pessimistic explanatory style still matter?". Personality and Individual Differences. 43 (5): 1149–59. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2007.03.007.
  26. ^ Welbourne, J.L.; Eggerth, D.; Hartley, T.A.; Andrew, M.E.; Sanchez, F. (2007). "Coping strategies in the workplace: Relationships with attributional style and job satisfaction". Journal of Vocational Behavior. 70 (2): 312–25. doi:10.1016/j.jvb.2006.10.006.
  27. ^ Bennett, K.K.; Elliott, M. (2005). "Pessimistic explanatory style and Cardiac Health: What is the relation and the mechanism that links them?". Basic and Applied Social Psychology. 27 (3): 239–48. doi:10.1207/s15324834basp2703_5. S2CID 144324529.
  28. ^ "Why Abuse Can Create Learned Helplessness".
  29. ^ Stipek, D.E.P. (1988). Motivation to learning. Allyn & Bacon: Boston.
  30. ^ Ramirez, E.; Maldonado, A.; Martos, R. (1992). "Attribution modulate immunization against learned helplessness in humans". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 62: 139–46. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.62.1.139.
  31. ^ Donovan, W.L.; Leavitt, L.A.; Walsh, R.O. (1990). "Maternal self-efficacy: Illusory control and its effect on susceptibility to learned helplessness". Child Development. 61 (5): 1638–47. doi:10.2307/1130771. JSTOR 1130771. PMID 2245753.
  32. ^ Rodin, J. (1986). "Aging and health: Effects of the sense of control". Science. 233 (4770): 1271–6. Bibcode:1986Sci...233.1271R. doi:10.1126/science.3749877. PMID 3749877.
  33. ^ Cox, William T. L.; Abramson, Lyn Y.; Devine, Patricia G.; Hollon, Steven D. (2012). "Stereotypes, Prejudice, and Depression The Integrated Perspective" (PDF). Original Paper. 7 (5): 427–449. doi:10.1177/1745691612455204. PMID 26168502. S2CID 1512121. Archived from the original (PDF) on 21 March 2014.
  34. ^ idem, p433
  35. ^ Payne, Ruby K. Framework for understanding poverty. Highlands, Tex: Aha! Process, 2005.
  36. ^ Young, L.D.; Allin, J.M. (1986). "Persistence of learned helplessness in humans". Journal of General Psychology. 113 (1): 81–8. doi:10.1080/00221309.1986.9710544. PMID 3701307.
  37. ^ Altmaier, E.M.; Happ, D.A. (1985). "Coping skills training's immunization effects against learned helplessness". Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology. 3 (2): 181–9. doi:10.1521/jscp.1985.3.2.181.
  38. ^ Thornton, J.W.; Powell, G.D. (1974). "Immunization to and alleviation of learned helplessness in man". American Journal of Psychology. 87 (3): 351–67. doi:10.2307/1421378. JSTOR 1421378.
  39. ^ Orbach, E.; Hadas, Z. (1982). "The elimination of learned helplessness deficits as a function of induced self-esteem". Journal of Research in Personality. 16 (4): 511–23. doi:10.1016/0092-6566(82)90009-5.
  40. ^ Norman, Donald (1988). The Design of Everyday Things. New York: Basic Books. pp. 41–42. ISBN 978-0-465-06710-7.
  41. ^ "How To Rid Your Primary Computing Classes Of 'Learned Helplessness'". Teachwire. Retrieved 4 January 2021.
  42. ^ Fleming, Sean (7 May 2021). "The Unabomber and the origins of anti-tech radicalism". Journal of Political Ideologies: 1–19. doi:10.1080/13569317.2021.1921940. ISSN 1356-9317.
  43. ^ "KUBARK COUNTERINTELLIGENCE INTERROGATION". CIA. July 1963. pp. Chapter IX. Coercive Counterintelligence Interrogation of Resistant Sources. Archived from the original on 2 June 2006. Retrieved 11 December 2014. If the debility-dependency-dread state is unduly prolonged, however, the arrestee may sink into a defensive apathy from which it is hard to arouse him.
  44. ^ Benedict Carey (10 December 2014). "Architects of C.I.A. Interrogation Drew on Psychology to Induce 'Helplessness'". The New York Times. The Times Company. Retrieved 11 December 2014.

External links[]

Listen to this article (9 minutes)
Menu
0:00
Spoken Wikipedia icon
This audio file was created from a revision of this article dated 17 December 2006 (2006-12-17), and does not reflect subsequent edits.

Retrieved from ""