Mainstream Science on Intelligence
"Mainstream Science on Intelligence" was a public statement issued by a group of researchers in fields associated with intelligence testing. It was originally published in The Wall Street Journal on December 13, 1994, as a response to criticism of the book The Bell Curve by Richard Herrnstein and Charles Murray, which appeared earlier the same year.[1] The statement defended Herrnstein and Murray's controversial claims about race and intelligence.[2]
The statement was drafted by Linda Gottfredson, a professor of educational psychology at the University of Delaware. It was sent to 131 researchers whom Gottfredsen described as "experts in intelligence and allied fields". Of these, 52 signed the statement, 48 returned the request with an explicit refusal to sign, and 31 ignored the request.[3][4]
According to a 1996 response by former American Psychological Association president Donald Campbell, only ten of those who signed were actual experts in intelligence measurement.[5] The Southern Poverty Law Center reports that 20 of the signers were recipients of funding from the white-supremacist organization the Pioneer Fund, including Gottfredson herself.[4]
In subsequent years, both the substance and the interpretation of this letter have received widespread criticism from the scientific community.[6][7][5][8][9][10]
Background[]
Gottfredson was prompted to write the statement by what she considered to be "outdated, pseudoscientific notions of intelligence" promoted by critics of The Bell Curve in the controversy that followed the publication of the book. She contacted David Brooks at The Wall Street Journal, who was willing to publish a short statement signed by experts describing what he considered mainstream in the study of intelligence. Gottfredson drafted the statement, had it vetted by several researchers, and finally solicited signatures for it from experts in several disciplines, including anthropology, behavior genetics, mental retardation, neuropsychology, sociology, and various specialties in psychology. The experts invited to sign the statement were given no opportunity to revise it, nor was anyone told who else had been invited or who had already given his or her signature.[3]
The invitation to sign was sent to 131 researchers, of whom 100 responded by the deadline. The signature form asked whether the respondent would sign the statement, and if not, why not. 48 sent back the request with an explicit refusal to sign and 31 ignored the request.[4] According to Gottfredson, seven of those who explicitly refused to sign did so "because they thought the statement did not represent the mainstream, 11 because they did not know whether it did, and 30 for other reasons."[3] 52 respondents agreed to sign the statement.[3]
Statement[]
The letter to The Wall Street Journal set out 25 numbered points it called "conclusions regarded as mainstream among researchers on intelligence" and "fully described in the major textbooks, professional journals and encyclopedias in intelligence":[8]
- "Intelligence is a very general mental capability ... it reflects a broader and deeper capability for comprehending our surroundings ..."
- "Intelligence, so defined, can be measured, and intelligence tests measure it well. They are among the most accurate (in technical terms, reliable and valid) of all psychological tests and assessments."
- "While there are different types of intelligence tests, they all measure the same intelligence."
- "The spread of people along the IQ continuum ... can be represented well by the ... ‘normal curve'."
- "Intelligence tests are not culturally biased"
- "The brain processes underlying intelligence are still little understood"
- "Members of all racial-ethnic groups can be found at every IQ level...The bell curves for some groups (Jews and East Asians) are centered somewhat higher than for whites in general. Other groups (blacks and Hispanics) are centered somewhat lower than non-Hispanic whites."
- "The bell curve for whites is centered roughly around IQ 100; the bell curve for American blacks roughly around 85; and those for different subgroups of Hispanics roughly midway between those for whites and blacks. The evidence is less definitive for exactly where above IQ 100 the bell curves for Jews and Asians are centered"
- "IQ is strongly related, probably more so than any other single measurable human trait, to many important educational, occupational, economic, and social outcomes ... Whatever IQ tests measure, it is of great practical and social importance"
- "A high IQ is an advantage because virtually all activities require some reasoning and decision-making"
- "The practical advantages of having a higher IQ increase as life’s settings become more complex"
- "Differences in intelligence certainly are not the only factor affecting performance in education, training, and complex jobs ... but intelligence is often the most important"
- "Certain personality traits, special talents, [etc] are important ... in many jobs, but they have narrower (or unknown) applicability or ‘transferability’ across tasks and settings compared with general intelligence"
- "Heritability estimates range from 0.4 to 0.8 ... indicating genetics plays a bigger role than environment in creating IQ differences"
- "Members of the same family also tend to differ substantially in intelligence"
- "That IQ may be highly heritable does not mean that it is not affected by the environment ... IQs do gradually stabilize during childhood, however, and generally change little thereafter"
- "Although the environment is important in creating IQ differences, we do not know yet how to manipulate it"
- "Genetically caused differences are not necessarily irremediable"
- "There is no persuasive evidence that the IQ bell curves for different racial-ethnic groups are converging"
- "Racial-ethnic differences in IQ bell curves are essentially the same when youngsters leave high school as when they enter first grade ... black 17-year-olds perform, on the average, more like white 13-year-olds"
- "The reasons that blacks differ among themselves in intelligence appear to be the same as those for why whites ... differ among themselves"
- "There is no definitive answer as to why bell curves differ across racial-ethnic groups. The reasons for these IQ differences between groups may be markedly different from the reasons for why individuals differ among themselves within any particular group"
- "Racial-ethnic differences are somewhat smaller but still substantial for individuals from the same socio-economic backgrounds"
- "Almost all Americans who identify themselves as black have white ancestors – the white admixture is about 20% ... research on intelligence relies on self-classification into distinct racial categories"
- "The research findings neither dictate nor preclude any particular social policy, because they can never determine our goals. They can, however, help us estimate the likely success and side-effects of pursuing those goals via different means."
Response and criticism[]
A 1995 article by Joseph L. Graves & Amanda Johnson was highly critical of the scientific basis on which Mainstream Science on Intelligence rested. The article stated that the statements in Mainstream Science in Intelligence
...certainly fall in line with general Euro-American impressions of race and intelligence. The problem, however, is that none of the ideas these psychometricians support have any relation to legitimate scientific inference. Even though individuals like psychologist J. Philippe Rushton have published monumental treatises to support the claim of Nordic racial superiority, and despite the flamboyant approaches of Shockley, Jensen, Herrnstein, and Murray, there remain only two ways the psychometric syllogism can be deemed acceptable-either: (a) one has little or no knowledge of the broad areas of scientific method, statistical reasoning, population studies, quantitative genetics, developmental physiology, neurophysiology, environmental toxicology, sociology, educational psychology, economics, and history required to adequately comprehend the issues involved; or (b) one has no desire to examine the facts of this problem objectively.[6]
In a posthumous article in 1996, Donald T. Campbell, a former president of the American Psychological Association, included his own analysis of the Wall Street Journal statement, previously drafted as a letter to that newspaper.[7][5] Campbell first remarked that:
Of the 52 signatories, there were 10 whom I would regard as measurement experts. I do not have a list of those who were asked to sign and refused, but I know they included Lee Cronbach, Robert Sternberg, and myself.
He remarked that the rhetorical organization of points in the statement, inadvertently or deliberately, seemed to him to build up to the conclusion that the black-white racial gap had a genetic cause. He pointed out that already at point 5, no provision had been allowed for differences in educational opportunity. Later on in point 14, he judged that the statements on heritability had been made without mentioning that it was based on twin studies, where environmental opportunities had been excluded as possible factors. In point 23, he pointed out that it was not possible to compare children of black and white parents that were "equally" educated, because in these circumstances the opportunities in the quality of education, both before and at college, would differ. On point 25, Campbell remarked that Jensen had himself published policy recommendations concerning rote learning.[5][7]
As Hauser (2010) reports in his discussion of the editorial, there is no general agreement about what is meant by intelligence. The editorial gave the following general definition of intelligence:[11]
Intelligence is a very general mental capability that, among other things, involves the ability to reason, plan, solve problems, think abstractly, comprehend complex ideas, learn quickly, and learn from experience. It is not merely book-learning, a narrow academic skill, or test-taking smarts. Rather, it reflects a broader and deeper capability for comprehending our surroundings "catching on," "making sense" of things, or "figuring out" what to do.
Gottfredson (1997b) describes intelligence in her own article in the same volume less broadly as "the ability to deal with complexity." However, the article by Carroll (1997a), one of the signatories of the statement, reviewed the numerous attempts in the academic literature to define what was meant by intelligence and found that there was no agreement. He cites experts as describing intelligence as "the total intellectual repertoire of behavioral responses," "some general property or quality ... of the brain," "reaction-time and physiological measures," "many different information-processing abilities" and "the rate with which learning occurs or the time required for learning." Plomin & Petrill (1997c) in the same volume describe intelligence as what is measured by intelligence tests: "What we mean by intelligence is general cognitive functioning (g) as assessed in the psychometric tradition of a general factor derived from a battery of diverse cognitive ability tests."
Harrington (1997) gives a point-by-point analysis of the conclusions of the letter. He points out that the validity of tests is claimed as a property of the tests, rather than how or where they are used. He questioned the letter's claims about there being no cultural bias in IQ tests; and that intelligence was a key factor determining fitness in human evolution, something that he argued was inconsistent with the claims of heritability, if the geneticist's version of evolution was being used. Harrington argued that heritability is phylogenetic, not ontogenetic as the letter suggested. He pointed out that the use of the term "race" differed from the way geneticists classify population groups. Regarding the assertion that IQ research does not preclude or dictate any particular social policy, Harrington commented that the views of Charles Murray on social policy have been used by US Congressmen to argue for policy changes.
The validity of the "mainstream science" described in the editorial was also questioned by Belkhir & Duyme (1998), who argued that the signatories of the were attempting to revive the "pseudo-scientific inquiry" of biological determinism.
In a 2001 article in Trends in Cognitive Sciences, Ian J. Deary noted that The Wall Street Journal "... is an odd place for such a document, and readers might view the signatories as one-sided, largely committed to the psychometrics-based intelligence research they were endorsing."[12]
Alderfer (2003) analysed the editorial as one of five responses to The Bell Curve, a book which he viewed as "an attempt to influence both psychological knowledge and U.S. politics". He concluded that some of the responses, including the editorial, "fell far short of providing a critical analysis of the book's racially biased argument and did little to reduce the misleading picture of race and IQ that the book promulgated." More specifically, Alderfer criticized the failure of the psychologists to recognize the effect of such a book on race relations in the US; as well as their failure to discuss the third and last part of the book on the implications for social policy. He wrote that, "Some psychologists said they wanted to keep themselves out of the emotional turmoil that had been generated by publication of the Bell Curve ... They might also have wanted to preserve the neutrality of psychology as a science. When examined in the contemporary racial context, however, their action was neither scientifically nor politically neutral. Essentially, they took a stand by not taking a stand. Their stand was not to become involved in how their expertise might be used to affect people's lives ...they missed an opportunity to caution their readers about regressive forces affecting U.S. race relations and to locate the book within that context. They did not fully use the authority based on their expertise to prevent harm."
Schlinger (2003) argued:
With a few exceptions, the list of cosigners reads like a Who's Who of those theorists (e.g., Thomas J. Bouchard, Jr., John B. Carroll, Raymond B. Cattell, Hans Eysenck, Linda S. Gottfredson, Seymour W. Itzkoff, Arthur R. Jensen, Robert Plomin, J. Philippe Rushton and Vincent Sarich) who have continued Spearman's tradition of factor analyzing intelligence test scores to generate a theory of general intelligence — g — and some of whom (e.g., Thomas J. Bouchard, Robert Plomin) believe that behavior genetic research supports the conclusion that g is highly heritable, and others of whom (e.g., Arthur Jensen, J. Philippe Rushton, Seymour Itzkoff) have written highly emotionally charged articles arguing that the research supports the conclusion that group differences on intelligence tests reflect genetic differences.
Armour-Thomas (2003) argued that the statement's claim that IQ tests were unbiased is not accepted by some prominent researchers in psychometrics who have pointed out the problems with using tests on population groups with a substantively different cultural background from those for whom the test was originally designed.
Anthropologist Robert Wald Sussman criticized the statement in his 2014 book The Myth of Race, writing, "In this letter, the members of the new bigot brigade claimed that since as many as fifty-two scientists signed this letter, the contents of the book [i.e. the Bell Curve] and of the letter must be true. Using this logic, since the vast majority of anthropologists and other social scientists and of geneticists do not agree with the conclusions of this volume, does that mean it must be false? Obviously not. However, it is the actual science that began with Boas and his colleagues and that has continued to this date that makes these authors’ beliefs and approach untenable at any level."[13]
The Southern Poverty Law Center states that some of the editorial's signatories "...had no relevant qualifications at all. Garrett Hardin, for example, was an ecologist and anti-immigration activist, while Vincent Sarich was an anthropologist who gained notoriety for making racist and homophobic claims in his undergraduate courses (he later admitted to The New York Times that these assertions were not based on established scientific facts)." After citing Campbell's estimate that only 10 signatories were experts on the measurement of intelligence, the Center noted that "...at least 20 [signatories] were recipients of money from the Pioneer Fund, including one director of the Fund, R. Travis Osborne, and two future presidents, J. Philippe Rushton and Richard Lynn."[4]
In a 2015 interview, behavior geneticist Robert Plomin was asked whether he regretted signing the statement. He reiterated his support for the factual assertions in the letter, but rejected interpretations of that data in The Bell Curve:
Well, I regret it to the extent it’s a distraction to my research. But I think the basic facts are there...erm, about heritability of intelligence, and it’s just so unfortunate that some of the interpretations [Murray and Herrnstein] made from that data are so, you know, some of them are quite bizarre and I would make just the opposite sorts of interpretations.[14]
Signatories[]
- Richard D. Arvey, University of Minnesota
- Thomas J. Bouchard, Jr., University of Minnesota
- John B. Carroll, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill
- Raymond B. Cattell, University of Hawaii
- David B. Cohen, University of Texas at Austin
- Rene V. Dawis, University of Minnesota
- Douglas K. Detterman, Case Western Reserve University
- Marvin Dunnette, University of Minnesota
- Hans Eysenck, University of London
- Jack M. Feldman, Georgia Institute of Technology
- Edwin A. Fleishman, George Mason University
- Grover C. Gilmore, Case Western Reserve University
- Robert A. Gordon, Johns Hopkins University
- Linda S. Gottfredson, University of Delaware
- Robert L. Greene, Case Western Reserve University
- Richard J. Haier, University of California, Irvine
- Garrett Hardin, University of California, Santa Barbara
- Robert Hogan, University of Tulsa
- Joseph M. Horn, University of Texas at Austin
- Lloyd G. Humphreys, University of Illinois at Urbana–Champaign
- John E. Hunter, Michigan State University
- Seymour W. Itzkoff, Smith College
- Douglas N. Jackson, University of Western Ontario
- James J. Jenkins, University of South Florida
- Arthur R. Jensen, University of California, Berkeley
- Alan S. Kaufman, University of Alabama
- Nadeen L. Kaufman, California School of Professional Psychology at San Diego
- Timothy Z. Keith, Alfred University
- Nadine Lambert, University of California, Berkeley
- John C. Loehlin, University of Texas at Austin
- David Lubinski, Iowa State University
- David T. Lykken, University of Minnesota
- Richard Lynn, University of Ulster at Coleraine
- Paul E. Meehl, University of Minnesota
- R. Travis Osborne, University of Georgia
- Robert Perloff, University of Pittsburgh
- Robert Plomin, Institute of Psychiatry, London
- Cecil R. Reynolds, Texas A&M University
- David C. Rowe, University of Arizona
- J. Philippe Rushton, psychologist, University of Western Ontario
- Vincent Sarich, University of Auckland New Zealand
- Sandra Scarr, University of Virginia
- Frank L. Schmidt, University of Iowa
- Lyle F. Schoenfeldt, Texas A&M University
- James C. Sharf, George Washington University
- Herman Spitz, former director E.R. Johnstone Training and Research Center, Bordentown, N.J.
- Julian C. Stanley, Johns Hopkins University
- Del Thiessen, University of Texas at Austin
- Lee A. Thompson, Case Western Reserve University
- Robert M. Thorndike, Western Washington University
- Philip Anthony Vernon, University of Western Ontario
- Lee Willerman, University of Texas at Austin
See also[]
- History of the race and intelligence controversy
- Snyderman and Rothman (study)
- The Bell Curve Debate
- Intelligence: Knowns and Unknowns
- Intelligence and How to Get It
- Superior: The Return of Race Science
Notes[]
- ^ McInerney, Joseph D. (March 1996). "Why Biological Literacy Matters: A Review of Commentaries Related to The Bell Curve: Intelligence and Class Structure in American Life". The Quarterly Review of Biology. 71 (1): 81–96. doi:10.1086/419269. JSTOR 3037831. S2CID 88113127.
- ^ Staub, Michael E. (8 May 2019). "The Mismeasure of Minds". Boston Review.
- ^ a b c d Gottfredson 1997, pp. 17–20
- ^ a b c d "Linda Gottfredson". Southern Poverty Law Center. Retrieved 2018-02-11.
- ^ a b c d Campbell 1996
- ^ a b Graves & Johnson 1995, pp. 279–280
- ^ a b c Laosa 1996
- ^ a b Harrington 1997, pp. 116–118
- ^ Alderfer (2003)
- ^ Armour-Thomas (2003)
- ^ Gottfredson 1997, p. 13
- ^ Deary 2001
- ^ Sussman 2014, p. 256
- ^ Gillborn 2016, p. 374
References[]
- Alderfer, C.P. (2003), "The science and nonscience of psychologists' responses to The Bell Curve", Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 34 (3): 287–293, doi:10.1037/0735-7028.34.3.287
- Armour-Thomas, Eleanor (2003), Assessment of psychometric intelligence for racial and ethnic minorities, Handbook of racial and ethnic minority psychology (ed. Guillermo Bernal), SAGE, pp. 357–374, ISBN 978-0-7619-1965-0
- Belkhir, Jean Ait; Duyme, Michel (1998), "Intelligence and Race, Gender, Class: The Fallacy of Genetic Determinism: Rethinking Intelligence From The Position Of The Oppressed", Race, Gender & Class, 5 (3): 136–176, JSTOR 41674879
- Campbell, Donald T. (1996), "Unresolved Issues in Measurement Validity : An Autobiographical Overview", Psychological Assessment, 8 (4): 363–368, doi:10.1037/1040-3590.8.4.363
- Carroll, J. B. (1997a), "Psychometrics, intelligence, and public perception", Intelligence, 24: 25–52, CiteSeerX 10.1.1.408.9146, doi:10.1016/S0160-2896(97)90012-X
- Deary, Ian J. (2001), "Human intelligence differences: a recent history", Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5 (3): 127–130, doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01621-1, PMID 11239813, S2CID 45099037
- Gillborn, David (2016), "Softly, softly: genetics, intelligence and the hidden racism of the new geneism" (PDF), Journal of Education Policy, 31 (4): 365–388, doi:10.1080/02680939.2016.1139189, S2CID 146275932
- Gottfredson, Linda S. (1997), "Mainstream Science on Intelligence (editorial)" (PDF), Intelligence, 24: 13–23, doi:10.1016/S0160-2896(97)90011-8
- Gottfredson, Linda S. (1997b), "Why g matters: The complexity of everyday life", Intelligence, 24: 79–132, CiteSeerX 10.1.1.535.4596, doi:10.1016/S0160-2896(97)90014-3
- Harrington, Gordon M. (1997), "Psychological testing, IQ, and evolutionary fitness", Genetica, 99 (2–3): 113–123, doi:10.1007/bf02259515, PMID 9463067, S2CID 9866526
- Graves, Joseph L.; Johnson, Amanda (1995), "The Pseudoscience of Psychometry and The Bell Curve", The Journal of Negro Education, 64 (3): 277–294, doi:10.2307/2967209, JSTOR 2967209
- Hauser, Robert M. (2010), "Causes and Consequences of Cognitive Functioning Across the Life Course" (PDF), Educational Researcher, 9 (2): 95–109, doi:10.3102/0013189x10363171, PMC 3289095, PMID 22383855, archived from the original (PDF) on 2011-08-10 (2009 AERA distinguished lecture)
- Laosa, Luis M. (1996), "Intelligence testing and social policy", Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 17 (2): 155–173, doi:10.1016/S0193-3973(96)90023-4
- Plomin, R.; Petrill, S. A. (1997c), "Genetics and intelligence: What's new?", Intelligence, 24: 53–77, doi:10.1016/s0160-2896(97)90013-1
- Schlinger, Henry D. (2003), "The myth of intelligence" (PDF), The Psychological Record, 53: 15–32
- Sussman, Robert W. (2014-10-06), The Myth of Race: The Troubling Persistence of an Unscientific Idea, Harvard University Press, ISBN 9780674417311
External links[]
- Original article
- Detterman, Douglas K. (January 1997). "Editor's note". Intelligence. 24 (1): iv. doi:10.1016/s0160-2896(97)90009-x. ISSN 0160-2896.
- Intelligence
- 1994 documents
- Race and intelligence controversy
- Scientific racism
- The Wall Street Journal