Cultivation theory

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Diagram summarizing the process of cultivation theory from a psychological point of view.[1]

Cultivation theory is a sociological and communications framework to examine the lasting effects of media, primarily television. It suggests that people who are regularly exposed to media for long periods of time are more likely to perceive the world's social realities as they are presented by the media they consume, which in turn affects their attitudes and behaviors.[2]

Cultivation theory was first advanced by professor George Gerbner in the 1960s;[3] it was later expanded upon by Gerbner and Larry Gross.[when?][4] Cultivation theory began as a way to test the impact of television on viewers, especially how exposure to violence through television affects human beings.[5] The theory's key proposition is that "the more time people spend 'living' in the television world, the more likely they are to believe social reality aligns with reality portrayed on television."[6] Because cultivation theory assumes the existence of objective reality and value-neutral research, it can be categorized as part of positivistic philosophy.[7]

In practice, images and ideological messages transmitted by popular media heavily influence perceptions of the real world. The more media that people consume, the more their perceptions change.[8][9] Such images and messages, especially when repeated, help bring about the culture that they portray. Cultivation theory aims to understand how long-term exposure to television programming, with its recurrent patterns of messages and images, can contribute to shared assumptions about the world.[citation needed]

In a 2004 study, surveying almost 2,000 articles published in the top three mass communication journals since 1956, Jennings Bryant and Dorina Miron found that cultivation theory was the third most frequently utilized cultural theory.[10]

Description[]

Cultivation theory suggests that exposure to media affects a viewer's perceptions of reality, drawing attention to three aspects: institutions, messages, and publics.[11]

Television, Gerbner suggested, binds diverse communities together by socializing people into standardized roles and behaviours; thus, television functions as part of the enculturation process.[12][13] Gerbner's research focused on the larger meaning of heavy television consumption instead of the meaning behind specific messages.[14]

A representation of the effects of mainstreaming and resonance. Source: Adapted from Gerber et al., “The mainstreaming of America: Violence profile no. 11” Journal of Communication, vol. 7 (1980), p. 16, Figure 2. Reprinted by permission of John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.[15]

Mainstreaming is the process by which TV viewers from disparate groups develop a common outlook of the world through exposure to the same images and labels,[13][16] the effect being stronger among those whose TV viewing is more constant.[8]

  • Blurring refers to the fusion of traditional distinctions,
  • Blending refers to the emergence of new conceptions into television's cultural mainstream, and
  • Bending refers to shifting the mainstream to the institutional interests of the medium and its sponsors.[16]

Resonance occurs when things viewed on television are congruent with the actual lived realities of viewers.[17]

Perceptions of Violence[]

Gerbner's initial work looked specifically at the effects of television violence on American audiences.[18] Measuring the effect of violence underscored the larger part of Gerbner's work on cultivation theory. Therefore, he measured dramatic violence, defined as "the overt expression or threat of physical force as part of the plot."[13] Gerbner's research also focused on the interpretation by high-use viewers of the prevalence of crime on television versus reality. He argues that, since a high percentage of programs include violent or crime-related content, viewers who spend a lot of time watching are inevitably exposed to high levels of crime and violence.[19]

In 1968, Gerbner conducted a survey to demonstrate this theory.[citation needed] Following his previous results, he placed television viewers into three categories: light viewers (less than 2 hours a day), medium viewers (2–4 hours a day), and heavy viewers (more than 4 hours a day). He found that heavy viewers held beliefs and opinions similar to those portrayed on television, which demonstrated the compound effect of media influence.[14] Heavy viewers experienced shyness, loneliness, and depression much more than those who watched less often.[20] From this study, Gerbner began work on what would become the Mean World Index, which subscribes to the notion that the heavy consumption of violence-related content leads the viewer to believe the world is more dangerous than it actually is.[citation needed]

In 2012, people with heavy viewing habits were found to believe that 5% of society was involved in law enforcement. In contrast, people with light viewing habits estimated a more realistic 1%.[13]

TV viewing and fear of crime[]

In most of the surveys conducted by Gerbner, the results revealed a small but statistically significant relationship between television consumption and fear of becoming the victim of a crime. Those with light viewing habits predicted their weekly odds of being a victim were 1 in 100; those with heavy viewing habits predicted 1 in 10. Actual crime statistics showed the risk to be 1 in 10,000.[13]

Supporting this finding is a survey done with college students that showed a significant correlation between the attention paid to local crime and one's fearfulness. There was also a significant correlation between fear of crime and violence and the number of times the respondents viewed television per week.[21]

Local news has been frequently analyzed for cultivation, as they rely "heavily on sensational coverage of crime and other mayhem with particular emphasis on homicide and violence", while news agencies boast of their allegiance to report factually.[22] Gerbner found that heavy viewers of news were more likely to overestimate crime rates and risk of personal exposure to crime and underestimate the safety of their neighborhoods.[23]Additionally, several other studies point out the correlation between viewing local news and fear of crime,[24][25] with Gross and Aiday (2003) finding the relationship between local news exposure and fear of crimes to be independent of local crime rates.[26]

Aside from local news, national news,[27] police shows,[24] and general TV news viewing[28] are also related to a fear of crime. Additionally, non-genre-specific TV viewing has been associated with fear of crime.[29][30]

Busselle (2003) found that parents who watch more programs portraying crime and violence are more likely to warn their children about crime during their high school years; these warnings, in turn, increased the students' own crime estimates,[31] suggesting cultivation takes place through both direct and indirect processes.

Criticism[]

Jennings Bryant points out that cultivation research focuses more on the effects rather than who or what is being influenced, being more to do with the whys and hows, as opposed to gathering normative data as to the whats, whos, and wheres.[32] Daniel Chandler argues that while television does have some effect on how we perceive the world around us, Gerbner's study does not consider the lived experiences of those who do inhabit high crime areas.[33] Horace Newcomb argues that violence is not presented as uniformly on television as the theory assumes; therefore, television cannot be responsible for cultivating the same sense of reality for all viewers.[34]

Background[]

In 1968, the National Commission on the Causes and Prevention of Violence was formed to address issues regarding violence in American culture, including racial injustice culminating in the assassination of Martin Luther King Jr, as well as the assassinations of Robert and John F. Kennedy.[citation needed] One area of interest for the administration of Lyndon Johnson was the effect of television violence on audiences.

George Gerbner began work on a federally-funded project at the Annenberg School of Communications.[35] In 1972, Congress facilitated the creation of the Surgeon General's Scientific Advisory Committee on Television and Social Behavior, which funded many studies, including the CIP.[clarification needed][citation needed] His research found less violence in reality than in prime time television.[36]

Cultivation theory is based on three core assumptions:

  1. Medium: the first assumption is that television is fundamentally different from other forms of mass media.[37]
  2. Audience: cultivation theory does not predict what people will do after watching a violent program but rather posits a connection between people's fears of a violence-filled world and their exposure to violent programming. The exposure to violent programming leads to what Gerbner calls the Mean World Syndrome, the idea that long-term exposure to violent media will lead to a distorted view that the world is more violent than it is.[citation needed]
  3. Function and Effect: television's effects are limited because it is a part of a larger sociocultural system. Therefore, although the effects of watching television may increase or decrease at any point in time, its effect is consistently present.[37]

Research[]

Using message system analysis as a tool helps researchers study viewer perceptions of reality, their perceptions of the observable world,[38] and can evaluate transmitted media content.[12]

Many theorists have extended Gerbner's theory. Gerbner's research focused on TV violence, but current research examines a variety of factors. Childhood viewing may be associated with overall self-esteem in children,[39] and affect one's beliefs as an adult.[40]

Studies outside the U.S., where the programming is less homogeneous and repetitive, produced results that are less consistent.[41] Australian students who watched US television programs (especially adventure and crime shows) were more likely to view Australia as dangerous;[42] however, they didn't perceive America as dangerous, even though they were watching US programs.

The cultivation effect is not specific to genre or program,[43] but can result from cumulative exposure to stable patterns of content on television.[35] Jonathan Cohen and Gabriel Weimann found cultivation more prevalent among teenagers and young adults, who may then exhibit cultivation longevity.[44]

Viewers tended toward greater psychosocial health when watching no more than 2 hours of television each day, following recommendations by the American Academy of Pediatrics (AAP), with an even greater impact on women. One study examined possible effects of viewing alcohol consumption in music videos."[45]

Another study looked at interactive video games and found the viewer's role within the game is essential in the progression of the story. Participants interacted with other players in real-time, with a strong showing of the cultivation of participants.[46]

Gerbner et al. developed an index for mean world syndrome. Those with heavy viewing habits become suspicious of other people's motives, expecting the worst.[13] For exampled, heavy viewers were much more likely to be afraid of walking alone at night.[19]

Heavy viewers[]

Heavy viewers are individuals who watch at least four hours of television a day.[13] However, Nielsen defined it as watching more than 11 hours a day.[47] Heavy viewers are consistently characterized as being more susceptible to images and messages. They also rely more on television to cultivate their perceptions of the real world.[35] In a recent study done on the cultivation effects of reality television, an Indiana University study found that young girls who regularly watched the MTV show Teen Mom had an unrealistic view of teen pregnancy.[48]

Several cognitive mechanisms that explain cultivation effects have been put forth by Shrum (1995, 1996, 1997).[49][50][51] Shrum's availability-heuristic explanation suggests that heavy viewers tend to retain more vivid memories of instances of television reality and more readily access those memories when surveyors ask them questions, resulting in more responses related to viewing, more quickly given. Another mechanism that might explain the cultivation phenomenon is a cognitive-narrative mechanism. Previous research suggests that the realism of narratives in combination with individual-level "transportability", or the ability to be less critical of a narrative, might facilitate cultivation effects.[52]

Dramatic violence[]

Dramatic violence is the "overt expression or serious threat of physical force as part of the plot."[13]

Shows such as Law & Order SVU and CSI: Miami use murder to frame each episode of their shows, underscoring the presence of gratuitous and dramatic violence.[53] The idea of dramatic violence reinforces the relationship between fear and entertainment. Though death is being used as a plot point, it also functions to cultivate an image of looming violence.

Magic Bullet Theory[]

The magic bullet theory is a linear model of communication concerned with audiences directly influenced by mass media and the media's power over them.

It assumes that the media's message is a bullet fired from a media "gun" into the viewer's head.[54] Similarly, the hypodermic needle model uses the same idea of direct injection. It suggests that the media delivers its messages straight into a passive audience's body.[55]

Television reality[]

"Television reality" denotes the effects of such media on heavy viewers. Cultivation theory research seems to indicate that heavy viewing can result in this reality, a set of beliefs based on content rather than facts.[56] Generally, the beliefs of heavy viewers about the world are consistent with the repetitive and emphasized images and themes presented on television.[42] As such, heavy viewing cultivates a television-shaped world view.[57]

Even across diverse demographics, the amount of viewing can make a difference in terms of viewer conceptions of social reality.[58] For instance, sex-role stereotypes can be traced to the independent contribution of TV viewing, just as sex, age, class, and education contribute.[58] The amount of viewing time is the main element in creating television reality for the audience. According to Gerbner's research, the more time spent absorbing the world of television, the more likely people are to report perceptions of social reality that can be traced to television's most persistent representations of life and society.[58]

Since the 1960s, communication scholars have examined television's contributions to viewers' perceptions of a wide variety of issues. However, little effort has been made to investigate the influence of television on perceptions of social reality among adolescents.[59]

Research supports the concept of television reality arising from heavy viewing. According to Wyer and Budesheim, television messages or information (even when they are not necessarily considered truthful) can still be used by viewers to make social judgments. Furthermore, the information shown to be invalid may still inform an audience's judgments.[60]

Gender and sexuality[]

LGBT[]

Sara Baker Netzley (2010) conducted research similar to Gerbner's into the way that gay people were depicted on television. This study found that there was an extremely high level of sexual activity in comparison to the number of gay characters that appeared on television. This has led those who are heavy television consumers to believe that the gay community is extremely sexual.[61] Much like the idea of a mean and scary world, it gives people an exaggerated sense of a sexualized gay community.[61]

A study conducted by Jerel Calzo and Monique Ward (2009) begins by analysing recent research conducted into the portrayal of gay and lesbian characters on television. While the representation of gay and lesbian characters has continued to grow, the study found that most television shows frame gay and lesbian characters in a manner that reinforces LGBT stereotypes. Diving into the discussion, Calzo and Ward describe even shows such as Ellen and Will & Grace as having storyline content that reinforces "stereotypes by portraying [...] characters as lacking stable relationships, as being preoccupied with their sexuality (or not sexual at all), and by perpetuating the perception of gay and lesbian people as laughable, one-dimensional figures." Their findings confirmed that media genres played an important role in forming attitudes regarding homosexuality. They were surprised by the finding that prior primetime shows, which are no longer on air, reinforced greater acceptance within the LGBTQ realm. They then suggested that, because genre played a large role in the perceptions that viewers formed while watching certain television shows, research should focus on "more genre-driven effects analyses."[62]

Women[]

Beverly Roskos-Ewoldsen, John Davies, and David Roskos-Ewoldsen (2004) posit that perceptions of women are integrated in a rather stereotypical fashion, compared to portrayals of men, on television. They state that, "men are characters in TV shows at about a 2 to 1 ratio to women." Viewers who consume more television usually also have more traditional views of women.[63] Research has also shown that women are more likely to be portrayed as victims on television than men.[28]

Alexander Sink and Dana Mastro (2017) studied women and gender depictions on American prime-time television. Although women are often perceived to be better represented on television in recent years, these researchers claim that this is not necessarily the case. They claim women are proportionally underrepresented on prime time television, making up 39% of characters even though women make up 50.9% of the population in the US. Men were also portrayed as more dominant than women, and although men were more often objectified, women were consistently portrayed as hyper-feminized and hyper-sexualized. Fewer older women appeared during prime time, compared to men, and were often shown to be less competent than older male characters.[64]

Sexual Attitudes[]

A study by Bradley J. Bond and Kristin L. Drogos (2014) examined the relationship between exposure to the television program Jersey Shore and sexual attitudes and behaviour in college-aged adults. They found a positive relationship between time spent watching Jersey Shore and increased sexual permissiveness. This effect was found to be stronger in the younger participants than in older ones and held true even when the researchers controlled for other influences on participants' sexual attitudes, such as religious beliefs and parents' attitudes. This higher level of sexually permissive attitudes and behaviour was not a result of higher overall exposure to television, but specifically to greater exposure to Jersey Shore, a highly sexualized program.[65]

Race and ethnicity[]

Meghan S. Sanders and Srividya Ramasubramanian (2012) studied perceptions that African American media consumers hold about fictional characters portrayed in film and television. They found that—while study participants tended to view all African American characters positively—social class, rather than race or ethnicity, mattered more in perceptions about the warmth and competence of a character. Their study suggests that the ethnicity of media consumers needs to be taken into account in cultivation studies, because media consumers with different backgrounds likely perceive media portrayals, and their faithfulness to reality, differently.[66]

A study by Elizabeth Behm-Morawitz and David Ta (2014) examined the cultivation effects of video games on White students' perceptions of Black and Asian individuals. While no significant effects were found for perceptions of Asian individuals, researchers found that those who spent a greater amount of time playing video games, no matter what genre, held a less positive view of Black people. They also found that real-life interaction with Black individuals did not change this effect. Behm-Morawitz and Ta suggest that the stable, negative racial and ethnic stereotypes portrayed in video game narratives of any genre impact real-world beliefs, in spite of more varied real-life interactions with racial and ethnic minorities.[67]

Politics and policy preferences[]

Diana Mutz and Lilach Nir (2010) conducted a study of how fictional television narratives can influence viewers' policy preferences and positive or negative attitudes regarding the justice system in the real world. They found that positive portrayals of the criminal justice system were associated with viewers' more positive view of the system in real life, whereas negative television portrayals were associated with viewers' feeling that the criminal justice system often works unfairly. Furthermore, researchers found that these attitudes did influence viewers' policy preferences concerning the criminal justice system in real life.[68]

A study by Anita Atwell Seate and Dana Mastro (2016) studied news coverage of immigration and its relationship to viewers' immigration policy preferences and negative attitudes regarding immigrants. They found that exposure to negative messages about immigrants in the news generated anxious feelings towards the outgroup (i.e., immigrants), particularly when the news program showed a member of the outgroup. This exposure did not necessarily immediately influence immigration policy preferences, but long-term exposure to messages of this kind can affect such preferences.[69]

Katerina-Eva Matsa (2010) explored cultivation effects through her thesis on television's impact on political engagement in Greece. She did so by describing the role of satirical television within the cultural realm in Greece and how this form of television created ingrained perceptions that Greek political institutions are corrupt, thus negatively influencing the Greek public's overall opinion of politics in their country.[70]

New media[]

Michael Morgan, James Shanahan, and Nancy Signorielli (2015) applied cultivation theory to the study of new media. They noted that media technology has not been static, and may continue to evolve. Therefore, older methods of cultivation analysis may have to move from counting hours of television viewed, and take a big data approach. These authors argue that, although many were skeptical that cultivation theory would be applicable with the increasing importance of new media, these new media still use narrative; and, since those narratives affect us, cultivation theory is still relevant.[71]

Stephen M. Croucher (2011) applied cultivation theory to his theory of social media and its effects on immigrant cultural adaptation. He theorizes that immigrants who use the dominant social media while they are still in the process of adapting to their new culture will develop perceptions about their host society from that media. He believes that this cultivation effect will also impact the way immigrants interact offline with natives of the host country.[72]

Sports[]

Cultivation theory attempts to predict that media viewing influences the values and beliefs that people have and the things they believe are "reality". A study conducted by the University of Connecticut's David Atkin revealed insights about television viewing of sports and the values of its viewers. The hypothesis stated that the "Level of agreement with sports-related values (i.e., being physically fit, athletic, and active) is positively related to participation in sports-related media and leisure activities."[73][citation needed] The results from the study supported the hypothesis, specifically that "those for whom being physically fit, being athletic and being active are important are exposed to more sports media."[citation needed] In this instance, cultivation theory is present because heavier exposure is related to greater agreement with the values that the media present.

An article titled "Sport in Society: Cultures, Commerce, Media and Politics",[full citation needed] concluded that "the line of research has found that, as exposure to television increases, an individual's beliefs and opinions of the real-world become more similar to that of the television world." This statement proves to be in support of the previous hypothesis and supports cultivation theory being present in the sporting world, in the sense that the more sport-related media that someone consumes, the more likely they are to value being physically fit.[citation needed]

Another aspect of cultivation theory being studied in relation to sports is the difference between those who participate in sporting events and those who watch them. Cultivation theory can explain people being less active and the rise in obesity levels, because of what they watch on television. Because people don't see many active people on television, their "reality" is that people no longer need to be active 30 or so minutes per day.[citation needed]

Cultivation theory can be applied to America's shift toward so-called "violent sports".[citation needed] A survey undertaken in 1998 showed that only 67% of American teenagers considered themselves to be baseball fans, compared to 78% who responded, identifying themselves as football fans.[citation needed] This study correlates with current TV ratings, as since 2005, football has by far the most hours watched, at 111.9 million hours.[citation needed] Leo W. Jeffres, Jae-Won Lee, and Kimberly A. Neuendorf[full citation needed] say that a "new 'media logic' that favors more violent, action-oriented sports" has emerged, "while slower-paced sports have been relegated to secondary status in the United States."

Although there was no true correlation between cultivation theory and sports, there has been research conducted on the level of violence in sports content and the effects it has on viewers. Results by Raney and Depalma (2006) found that individuals were less likely to report being in a positive mood after watching violent sports content.[full citation needed]

Altruistic behaviour[]

Zakir Shah, Jianxun Chu, Usman Ghani, Sara Qaisar, and Zameer Hassan (2020) conducted the first study from the perspective of cultivation theory to determine the mediating role of fear of victimization, gained from exposure to disaster-related media, on altruistic behaviour. Findings show that exposure to disaster-related news and individuals’ perceptions taken from the media contributed to fear of victimization. Moreover, fear of victimization from disaster significantly influences the altruistic behaviour of people.[74]

See Also[]

References[]

  1. ^ Bilandzic, Helena; Rössler, Patrick (2004-01-12). "Life according to television. Implications of genre-specific cultivation effects: The Gratification/Cultivation model". Communications. 29 (3). doi:10.1515/comm.2004.020. ISSN 0341-2059.
  2. ^ Nabi, Robin L.; Riddle, Karyn (2008-08-08). "Personality Traits, Television Viewing, and the Cultivation Effect". Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media. 52 (3): 327–348. doi:10.1080/08838150802205181. ISSN 0883-8151. S2CID 144333259.
  3. ^ Communication, in Cultural; Psychology; Behavioral; Science, Social (2010-01-14). "Cultivation Theory". Communication Theory. Retrieved 2021-02-05.
  4. ^ "Cultivation Theory by George Gerbner & Larry Gross". toolshero. 2019-05-09. Retrieved 2021-01-25.
  5. ^ Settle, Quisto (2018-11-05). "Introducing Communication Theory: Analysis and Application". Journal of Applied Communications. 102 (3). doi:10.4148/1051-0834.1223. ISSN 1051-0834.
  6. ^ Riddle, K. (2009). Cultivation Theory Revisited: The Impact of Childhood Television Viewing Levels on Social Reality Beliefs and Construct Accessibility in Adulthood (Conference Papers). International Communication Association. pp. 1–29.
  7. ^ West, R. & Turner, L. H. (2010). Introducing Communication Theory: Analysis and Application (Fourth ed.). New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
  8. ^ Jump up to: a b Gerbner, George (1987). "SCIENCE ON TELEVISION: How It Affects Public Conceptions". Issues in Science and Technology. 3 (3): 109–115. ISSN 0748-5492. JSTOR 43309074.
  9. ^ Ph.D, Michael Morgan; Ph.D, James Shanahan (2010-05-19). "The State of Cultivation". Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media. 54 (2): 337–355. doi:10.1080/08838151003735018. ISSN 0883-8151.
  10. ^ Bryant, J.; Mirion, D. (2004). "Theory and research in mass communication". Journal of Communication. 54 (4): 662–704. doi:10.1093/joc/54.4.662.
  11. ^ Morgan, M.; Shanahan, J. (2010). "The State of Cultivation". Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media. 54 (2): 337–355. doi:10.1080/08838151003735018. S2CID 145520112.
  12. ^ Jump up to: a b Gerbner, G.; Gross, L.; Morgan, M. & Signorielli, N. (1986). "Living with television: The dynamics of the cultivation process". In J. Bryant & D. Zillman (eds.). Perspectives on media effects. Hilldale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 17–40.
  13. ^ Jump up to: a b c d e f g h Griffin, E. (2012). Communication Communication Communication. New York: McGraw-Hill. pp. (8), 366–377.
  14. ^ Jump up to: a b Potter, W. James (1 December 2014). "A Critical Analysis of Cultivation Theory". Journal of Communication. 64 (6): 1015–1036. doi:10.1111/jcom.12128. ISSN 1460-2466. S2CID 143285507.
  15. ^ Gerbner, George; Gross, Larry; Morgan, Michael; Signorielli, Nancy (1980-09-01). "The "Mainstreaming" of America: Violence Profile No. 11". Journal of Communication. 30 (3): 10–29. doi:10.1111/j.1460-2466.1980.tb01987.x. ISSN 0021-9916.
  16. ^ Jump up to: a b Perse, E. M. (2005-03-01). "Against the Mainstream: The Selected Works of George Gerbner: Edited by Michael Morgan. New York: Peter Lang, 2002. 528 pp. $73.95 (hard), $29.95 (soft)". Journal of Communication. 55 (1): 187–188. doi:10.1093/joc/55.1.187. ISSN 0021-9916.
  17. ^ Gerbner, George; Gross, Larry; Morgan, Michael; Signorielli, Nancy (1980-09-01). "The "Mainstreaming" of America: Violence Profile No. 11". Journal of Communication. 30 (3): 10–29. doi:10.1111/j.1460-2466.1980.tb01987.x. ISSN 0021-9916.
  18. ^ "Media Effects Theories". Oregon State University.
  19. ^ Jump up to: a b Hughes, Michael (1980). "The Fruits of Cultivation Analysis: A Reexamination of Some Effects of Television Watching". Public Opinion Quarterly. 44 (3): 287–302. doi:10.1086/268597.
  20. ^ Hammermeister, Joe; Barbara Brock; David Winterstein; Randy Page (2005). "Life Without TV? Cultivation Theory and Psychosocial Characteristics of Television-Free Individuals and Their Television-Viewing Counterparts" (PDF). Health Communication. 17 (4): 253–264. doi:10.1207/s15327027hc1703_3. PMID 15855072. S2CID 18565666.
  21. ^ Reber, Bryan H.; Yuhmim Chang (1 September 2000). "Assessing cultivation theory and public health model for crime reporting". Newspaper Research Journal. 21 (4): 99–112. doi:10.1177/073953290002100407. S2CID 152903384.
  22. ^ Hamilton, 1998; Klite, Bardwell, & Salzman, 1995, 1997[full citation needed]
  23. ^ 60.Lett, M. D., DiPietro, A. L., & Johnson, D. I. (2004). Examining Effects of Television News Violence on College Students through Cultivation Theory. Communication Research Reports, 21(1), 39–46.
  24. ^ Jump up to: a b Eschholz, Sarah; Chiricos, Ted; Gertz, Marc (2003-08-01). "Television and Fear of Crime: Program Types, Audience Traits, and the Mediating Effect of Perceived Neighborhood Racial Composition". Social Problems. 50 (3): 395–415. doi:10.1525/sp.2003.50.3.395. ISSN 0037-7791.
  25. ^ Weitzer, Ronald; Kubrin, Charis E. (2004-09-01). "Breaking news: How local TV news and real-world conditions affect fear of crime". Justice Quarterly. 21 (3): 497–520. doi:10.1080/07418820400095881. ISSN 0741-8825.
  26. ^ Gross, Kimberly; Aday, Sean (2003-09-01). "The Scary World in Your Living Room and Neighborhood: Using Local Broadcast News, Neighborhood Crime Rates, and Personal Experience to Test Agenda Setting and Cultivation". Journal of Communication. 53 (3): 411–426. doi:10.1111/j.1460-2466.2003.tb02599.x. ISSN 0021-9916.
  27. ^ Holbert, R. Lance; Shah, Dhavan V.; Kwak, Nojin (2016-06-25). "Fear, Authority, and Justice: Crime-Related TV Viewing and Endorsements of Capital Punishment and Gun Ownership". Journalism & Mass Communication Quarterly. 81 (2): 343–363. doi:10.1177/107769900408100208.
  28. ^ Jump up to: a b Romer, D.; Jamieson, K. H.; Aday, S. (2003). "Television news and the cultivation of fear of crime". Journal of Communication. 53 (1): 88–104. doi:10.1111/j.1460-2466.2003.tb03007.x. S2CID 16535313.
  29. ^ Bulck, Jan Van den (2016-07-26). "Research Note: the Relationship between Television Fiction and Fear of Crime: An Empirical Comparison of Three Causal Explanations". European Journal of Communication. doi:10.1177/0267323104042922.
  30. ^ NABI, ROBIN L.; SULLIVAN, JOHN L. (2016-06-30). "Does Television Viewing Relate to Engagement in Protective Action Against Crime?: A Cultivation Analysis From a Theory of Reasoned Action Perspective". Communication Research. doi:10.1177/009365001028006004.
  31. ^ Busselle, Rick W. (October 2003). "Television Exposure, Parents' Precautionary Warnings, and Young Adults' Perceptions of Crime". Communication Research. 30 (5): 530–556. doi:10.1177/0093650203256360. ISSN 0093-6502. S2CID 35569859.
  32. ^ Bryant, Jennings. (1 March 1986). "The Road Most Traveled: Yet Another Cultivation Critique". Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media. 30 (2): 231–335. doi:10.1080/08838158609386621.
  33. ^ Chandler, Daniel. Cultivation Theory Archived 2011-12-06 at the Wayback Machine. Aberystwyth University, 18 September 1995.
  34. ^ Newcomb, H (1978). "Assessing the violence profile studies of Gerbner and Gross: A humanistic critique and suggestion". Communication Research. 5 (3): 264–283. doi:10.1177/009365027800500303. S2CID 144063275.
  35. ^ Jump up to: a b c Shanahan, J; Morgan, M (1999). Television and its viewers: Cultivation theory and research. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. pp. 286–302. ISBN 978-1-4331-1368-0.
  36. ^ West, Richard; Turner, Lynn (2014). Introducing Communication Theory: Analysis and Application. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill Education. pp. 420–436. ISBN 978-0-07-353428-2.
  37. ^ Jump up to: a b Gerbner, G.; Gross, L.; Jackson-Beeck, M.; Jeffries-Fox, S.; Signorielli, N. (1978). "Cultural indicators violence profile no. 9". Journal of Communication. 28 (3): 176–207. doi:10.1111/j.1460-2466.1978.tb01646.x. PMID 690257. S2CID 34741270.
  38. ^ Gerbner, G. & Gross, L. (1972). "Living with television: The violence profile". Journal of Communication. 26 (2): 173–199. doi:10.1111/j.1460-2466.1976.tb01397.x. PMID 932235.
  39. ^ Martins, Nicole; Harrison, Kristen (16 March 2011). "Racial and Gender Differences in the Relationship Between Children's Television Use and Self-Esteem". Communication Research. 39 (3): 338–357. doi:10.1177/0093650211401376. S2CID 26199959.
  40. ^ Griffin, E. (2012). Communication Communication Communication. New York: McGraw-Hill. pp. (8), 366–377.
  41. ^ Gerbner G, Gross L, Morgan M, Signorielli N (1994). "Growing up with television: The cultivation perspective". In M. Morgan (ed.). Against the mainstream: The selected works of George Gerbner. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 193–213.
  42. ^ Jump up to: a b Gerbner G, Gross L, Morgan M, Signorielli N (1994). "Growing up with television: The cultivation perspective". In M. Morgan (ed.). Against the mainstream: The selected works of George Gerbner. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 193–213.
  43. ^ Quick. "The Effects of Viewing Grey's Anatomy on Perceptions of Doctors and Patient Satisfaction". Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media. Archived from the original on 9 June 2010.
  44. ^ Cohen, J. & Weimann, G. (2000). "Cultivation revisited: Some genres have some effects on some viewers". Communication Reports. 13 (2): 99–114. doi:10.1080/08934210009367728. S2CID 144833310.
  45. ^ Beullens, K.; Roe, K.; Van; den Bulck, J. (2012). "Music Video Viewing as a Marker of Driving After the Consumption of Alcohol". Substance Use & Misuse. 47 (2): 155–165. doi:10.3109/10826084.2012.637449. PMID 22217069. S2CID 32434210.
  46. ^ Williams, Dmitri (2006). "Virtual cultivation: Online worlds, offline perceptions". Journal of Communication. 56: 69–87. doi:10.1111/j.1460-2466.2006.00004.x. S2CID 4661334.
  47. ^ "Tipping the Scale: Heavy TV Viewers=a Big Opportunity for Advertisers". www.nielsen.com. Retrieved 24 November 2015.
  48. ^ "Study: Heavy viewers of 'Teen Mom' and '16 and Pregnant' have unrealistic views of teen pregnancy: IUB Newsroom: Indiana University". news.indiana.edu. Retrieved 24 November 2015.
  49. ^ Shrum, L.J. (1995). "Assessing the social influence of television: A social cognitive perspective on cultivation effects". Communication Research. 22 (4): 402–429. doi:10.1177/009365095022004002. S2CID 145114684.
  50. ^ Shrum, L.J. (1996). "Psychological processes underlying cultivation effects: Further tests of construct accessibility". Human Communication Research. 22 (4): 482–509. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2958.1996.tb00376.x.
  51. ^ Shrum, L.J. (1997). "The role of source confusion in cultivation effects may depend on processing strategy: A comment on Mares (1996)". Human Communication Research. 24 (2): 349–358. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2958.1997.tb00418.x.
  52. ^ Bilandzic, H.; Busselle, R.W (2008). "Transportation and transportability in the cultivation of genre-consistent attitudes and estimates". Journal of Communication. 58 (3): 508–529. doi:10.1111/j.1460-2466.2008.00397.x.
  53. ^ Berger, Arthur Asa, ed. (1987). Television in Society. Transaction Publishers. ISBN 978-113851528-4.
  54. ^ Arthur Asa, Berger (5 July 1995). Essentials of Mass Communication Theory (1 ed.). London: SAGE Publications.
  55. ^ Croteau, D.; Hoynes, W (1997). Industries and Audience. London: Pine Forge Press.
  56. ^ Tony R., DeMars (2000). Modeling Behavior from Images of Reality in Television Narratives. US: The Edwin Mellen Press, Ltd. p. 36.
  57. ^ Hawkins, R. P.; Pingree, S.; Alter, I (1987). Searching for cognitive processes in the cultivation effect: Adult and adolescent samples in the United States and Australia. pp. 553–577.
  58. ^ Jump up to: a b c Gerbner, George; Larry Gross; Nancy Signorielli; Michael Morgan (1980). "Aging with Television: Image on Television Drama and Conceptions of Social Reality". Journal of Communication. 30 (1): 37–47. doi:10.1111/j.1460-2466.1980.tb01766.x. PMID 7372841.
  59. ^ Jong G. Kang; Stephen S. Andersen; Michael Pfau (1996). "Television Viewing and Perception of Social Reality Among Native American Adolescents" (PDF). Illinois State University, Augustana College University of Wisconsin, Madison.
  60. ^ Wyer, Robert S.; William H. Unverzagt (1985). "Effects of Instructions to Disregard Information on Its Subsequent Recall and Use in Making Judgments". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 48 (3): 533–549. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.48.3.533. PMID 3989660.
  61. ^ Jump up to: a b Netzley, S (2010). "Visibility That Demystifies Gays, Gender, and Sex on Television". Journal of Homosexuality. 57 (8): 968–986. doi:10.1080/00918369.2010.503505. PMID 20818525. S2CID 5230589.
  62. ^ Jerel P. Calzo M.A. & L. Monique Ward (2009). "Media Exposure and Viewers' Attitudes Toward Homosexuality: Evidence for Mainstreaming or Resonance?". Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media. 53 (2): 280–299. doi:10.1080/08838150902908049. S2CID 144908119.
  63. ^ Roskos-Ewoldsen, Beverly; Davies, John; Roskos-Ewoldsen, David (2004). "Implications of the mental models approach for cultivation theory". Communications: The European Journal of Communication Research. 29 (3). doi:10.1515/comm.2004.022.
  64. ^ Sink, Alexander; Mastro, Dana (2 January 2017). "Depictions of Gender on Primetime Television: A Quantitative Content Analysis". Mass Communication and Society. 20 (1): 3–22. doi:10.1080/15205436.2016.1212243. ISSN 1520-5436. S2CID 151374826.
  65. ^ Bond, Bradley J.; Drogos, Kristin L. (2 January 2014). "Sex on the Shore: Wishful Identification and Parasocial Relationships as Mediators in the Relationship Between Jersey Shore Exposure and Emerging Adults' Sexual Attitudes and Behaviors". Media Psychology. 17 (1): 102–126. doi:10.1080/15213269.2013.872039. ISSN 1521-3269. S2CID 145263485.
  66. ^ Sanders, Meghan S.; Ramasubramanian, Srividya (1 January 2012). "An Examination of African Americans' Stereotyped Perceptions of Fictional Media Characters". Howard Journal of Communications. 23 (1): 17–39. doi:10.1080/10646175.2012.641869. ISSN 1064-6175. S2CID 144745770.
  67. ^ Behm-Morawitz, Elizabeth; Ta, David (1 January 2014). "Cultivating Virtual Stereotypes?: The Impact of Video Game Play on Racial/Ethnic Stereotypes". Howard Journal of Communications. 25 (1): 1–15. doi:10.1080/10646175.2013.835600. ISSN 1064-6175. S2CID 144462796.
  68. ^ Mutz, Diana C.; Nir, Lilach (31 March 2010). "Not Necessarily the News: Does Fictional Television Influence Real-World Policy Preferences?". Mass Communication and Society. 13 (2): 196–217. doi:10.1080/15205430902813856. ISSN 1520-5436. S2CID 143452861.
  69. ^ Seate, Anita Atwell; Mastro, Dana (2 April 2016). "Media's influence on immigration attitudes: An intergroup threat theory approach". Communication Monographs. 83 (2): 194–213. doi:10.1080/03637751.2015.1068433. ISSN 0363-7751. S2CID 146477267.
  70. ^ Matsa, K. E. (2010). Laughing at politics: effects of television satire on political engagement in Greece (PDF) (Thesis). Georgetown University.
  71. ^ Morgan, Michael; Shanahan, James; Signorielli, Nancy (3 September 2015). "Yesterday's New Cultivation, Tomorrow". Mass Communication and Society. 18 (5): 674–699. doi:10.1080/15205436.2015.1072725. ISSN 1520-5436. S2CID 143254238.
  72. ^ Croucher, Stephen M. (1 November 2011). "Social Networking and Cultural Adaptation: A Theoretical Model". Journal of International and Intercultural Communication. 4 (4): 259–264. doi:10.1080/17513057.2011.598046. ISSN 1751-3057. S2CID 143560577.
  73. ^ Atkin, David J. (2006). Communication Technology And Social Change. Routledge Communication Series. Routledge. p. 324. ISBN 978-080585614-9.
  74. ^ Shah, Zakir; Chu, Jianxun; Ghani, Usman; Qaisar, Sara; Hassan, Zameer (January 2020). "Media and altruistic behaviors: The mediating role of fear of victimization in cultivation theory perspective". International Journal of Disaster Risk Reduction. 42: 101336. doi:10.1016/j.ijdrr.2019.101336. ISSN 2212-4209.

Further reading[]

  • Gerbner, G.; Gross, L.; Morgan, M.; Signorielli, N.; Jackson-Beeck, M. (1979). "The Demonstration of Power: Violence Profile No. 10". Journal of Communication. 29 (3): 177–196. doi:10.1111/j.1460-2466.1979.tb01731.x. PMID 479387.
Retrieved from ""